Discoveries in Animal Behavior

| | Comments (6)

animal_behavior.jpg

Copyright Jessica Snyder Sachs
(first published in National Wildlife magazine)

JAMES HARE stands in a grassy Midwestern pasture, tossing a tan fedora over the head of a female gopher (a.k.a. Richardson’s ground squirrel) as she emerges from her burrow. Hare, a zoologist with the University of Manitoba, knows that the swooping hat will trigger the gopher’s instinctive alarm response. She flicks her tail, opens her mouth and heaves her chest as if to give the sharp chirrup that announces “Hawk!” to the surrounding colony. Instead, she appears struck dumb, nothing but a faint whisper of air rushing from her gaping mouth.

The first time Hare observed this behavior several years ago, he came away puzzled. His fascination grew as he noticed other gophers opening their mouths as if to bark a warning, only to exhale with a barely audible call. An outbreak of ground squirrel laryngitis, perhaps? Hare’s early, lost-voice theory crumbled when closer study revealed gophers switching back and forth between their usual alarm calls and the strange, open-mouthed whisper.

Like Hare, scholars as far back as the ancient Egyptians and Greeks have puzzled over mysteries like these. Today the study of animal behavior, or ethology, is still one of biology’s most productive fields, with scientific journals brimming with reports on everything from the selection of gender-specific parental roles in cichlid fish to the importance of courtship song in fruit flies (seriously).

While much of this scholarly minutiae holds little draw beyond specialist circles, every year scientists make discoveries that overturn widespread assumptions or reveal previously unknown animal abilities. Here are just few of the more recent noteworthy finds.

SILENT SCREAMS AND JUNGLE RUMBLES
Determined to solve the mystery of the whispering gophers, Hare returned to his university, where he borrowed an ultrasonic “bat detector.” Such devices lower high-frequency sounds—such as a bat’s echolocation shrieks—into human range by slowing down their pitch. Back out on the prairie, he pointed the device at emerging gophers as they reacted with visible but noiseless alarm to his presence. Sure enough, these were screams—at frequencies around 48,000 hertz. By contrast, the limits of human hearing reach only half as high, around 20,000 hertz. Hare suspected that he had discovered a “silent” alarm call.

To test the idea, graduate student David Wilson recorded silent screams from over a dozen gophers. He and Hare then replayed the recordings for gophers miles away. The ultrasonic cries produced much the same reactions as did recordings of audible alarms, with one clear difference. The ultrasonic recordings triggered alarm only in gophers close to and directly in front of the speakers.

The researchers’ conclusion: While the gopher’s silent alarm does not travel as far or wide as does the species’ familiar warning bark, a gopher can direct it, with laser-beam accuracy, to a nearby pup or other close relative, all without endangering itself by drawing attention from a predator. “This sort of thing may be far more common than we realize,” says Hare, who speculates that we live in a world filled with wildlife chatter beyond our senses.

That view gets support from a discovery at the opposite end of the sound spectrum. In Papua New Guinea, biologists from the New York-based Wildlife Conservation Society have recorded scarcely audible rumblings coming from the direction of large, ostrichlike birds called cassowaries. “It’s hard to describe, but you more feel it than hear it,” says study leader Andrew Mack.
He and his students have documented cassowaries producing “boom” calls at infrasound frequencies at the lower limit of human hearing—around 20 hertz and, by far, the deepest bird calls ever recorded. Mack suspects the solitary birds use their infrasonic booms to locate mates in the dense underbrush of the rain forest, where the vibration can carry for at least a mile.

NAVIGATION WONDERS
In the mid-1800s, scientists began to speculate that some migratory birds rely on an internal compass to guide their journeys along Earth’s magnetic fields. Since this idea was accepted in the 1970s, they’ve found evidence for similar compasses in a wide range of other animals, including crustaceans, insects, fish, amphibians and mammals.

More recently, zoologists have shown that, in some birds, this navigational sense goes beyond a mere compass by providing maplike information about position—akin to today’s global positioning systems. Since then, the race has been on to find similar abilities in animals outside the bird family.

Surprisingly, the first clear evidence has turned up in a lowly invertebrate: the spiny lobster, which migrates up to 120 miles across the Gulf of Mexico and Caribbean. To shed light on how the animals accomplish this feat on the bottom of the sea floor—without sunlight or other visual clues—University of North Carolina researchers began studying young lobsters in the coastal waters off the Florida Keys. After capturing the crustaceans, the scientists transported them (inside closed containers) along circuitous routes of about 20 miles to one of two test sites, one north and the other south of their home range. To ensure the animals could not use visual cues to orient themselves, the researchers placed rubber caps over the lobsters’ eyestalks. Despite this, the lobsters all began walking in the direction of home—be that north or south of their drop-off site.

To test whether the lobsters were using geomagnetic navigation, the researchers returned to the home site and used powerful magnets to simulate the geomagnetic field conditions that exist some 250 miles either north or south. Sure enough, the lobsters exposed to the northerly magnetic field immediately began walking south, just as those exposed to the southerly field turned to walk north.

JUST HUMAN NATURE?
Sarah Brosnan had long marveled at the apparent generosity of the capuchin monkeys she studies at Yerkes National Primate Research Institute in Atlanta, where the animals live in two large and highly social colonies. If Brosnan left a bowl of food within reach of one capuchin but not another—separating them with a mesh partition—the first invariably shared by passing food through the divider.

As an anthropologist, Brosnan has a special interest in the roots of such behavior, specifically in mechanisms that encourage humans to cooperate when they receive no immediate benefit. Is the Golden Rule something we learn, or might it stem from an instinctual sense of fair play that we share with other primates?

At Yerkes, Brosnan designed an experiment to explore the question. First, she taught the monkeys a bartering game: She would hand one of them a small stone, then offer it a grape or a cucumber slice in trade. The capuchins eagerly bartered for either treat, though they clearly preferred the sweeter fare.

Next, Brosnan began giving one capuchin a grape, offering its partner a cucumber. The monkeys’ reaction to such unequal treatment could only be described as outrage, says Brosnan. “They’d literally throw their cucumber slices at me, something I’d never seen in all the years I worked with them.” The capuchins reacted even more negatively if they witnessed another receive the preferred treat for less work—if Brosnan handed their partner a grape, for example, without receiving a token exchange. In that situation, those offered cucumbers often threw away their stones or turned their backs on the scientist.

“People often turn down a reward because it’s not what they think is fair,” notes Brosnan. “Such self-defeating behavior may not seem rational, but our research suggests it traces to the kind of emotional sense of fairness that may promote the high level of cooperation needed in species that hunt or otherwise work closely together.”

Elsewhere in the primate world, researchers have documented another eerie mirror of human behavior: Girl chimpanzees appear to study harder than do their rambunctious brothers. Working in Tanzania’s Gombe National Park, University of Minnesota scientists tracked how young chimps learn to fish for termites using tools they make from sticks and stems. While both sons and daughters accompanied their mothers on termite-fishing expeditions, the daughters spent far more time watching and imitating, while the more-easily-distracted sons spent most of the time wrestling with each other.

As a result, most young females had perfected the skill by 30 months of age, while their brothers didn’t catch on until they were nearly twice as old. That’s not to say the boys were wasting their time, says study leader Elizabeth Lonsdorf. Their rough-and-tumble play may be important to sorting out dominance—a key factor in a male’s later reproductive success.

Behavior Benders: Pollutants Tied to Behavioral Oddities
In the 1970s, ornithologists began noticing a dramatic increase in same-sex California gull couples, with an abundance of female pairs attending nests brimming with both birds’ eggs. While most of the gay gulls did just fine raising their chicks, thank you, the abrupt change in behavior proved an early warning of something amiss. In the following years, the gulls began to show signs of acute poisoning by the pesticide DDT.

Fast-forward to 2004, and a flood of reports from field biologists documenting a veritable freak show of bizarre animal behaviors, from fearless mice, stupid frogs and paranoid ducks to spastic egrets, hyperactive doves and sluggish salmon. Add to these an especially disturbing increase in clueless mating behaviors and neglectful parenting in species from crustaceans to kestrels.

In these and scores of other cases, biologists have now traced the eccentricities to low-level environmental contamination with behavior-bending chemicals known as endocrine disrupters. “We’ve documented that hundreds of man-made pollutants have profound effects on animal behavior,” reports Amherst University biologist Ethan Clotfelter.

These chemicals include hundreds of biologically active pesticides, heavy metals and pharmaceutical products spread by runoff and seepage from farms, factories and municipal sewer systems. Worse, the low-level contamination has now pervaded every known ecosystem, from the tropics to the Arctic Circle. But with environmental testing geared to monitor for acute toxicity—that is, animals falling down dead—these behavioral disruptions have gone largely unnoticed, says Clotfelter, whose report on scores of such behavioral effects appeared in a recent issue of the journal Animal Behavior.

Clotfelter and other researchers who study endocrine disrupters are urging field biologists and laboratory toxicologists to recognize even subtle changes in animal behavior as a red flag that a particular chemical or an environmental cocktail of contaminants is approaching deadly levels.

“Like the DDT-poisoned gulls in the 1970s, you may see behavioral effects long before you see a population crash,” he says. And the weird behaviors alone may threaten the survival of species already on the brink of extinction. Examples include the loss of predator-evasion instincts in endangered Pacific salmon in streams contaminated with trace levels of pesticides from bordering farm fields.

New Jersey journalist Jessica Snyder Sachs wrote about the natural history of water in the June/July 2004 issue. To read about more animal behavior discoveries, see this month’s “Web Exclusives” at www.nwf.org/nationalwildlife.

6 Comments

Informative and interesting which we share with you so i think so it is very useful and knowledgeable. I would like to thank you for the efforts. I am tiring the same best work from me in the future as well.
==================================
pass4sure 70-511
pass4sure 70-512
pass4sure 70-513
pass4sure 70-513
pass4sure 70-515
pass4sure 70-516
pass4sure 70-516
pass4sure 70-519

It is a very inspiring post to go through for excellent story about sustainability; food and environment in urban areas. The successful example of urban farming in Chicago can be followed in other cities to make the cities greener and less polluted. I've enjoyed reading the interesting discussion. I'll use this info to provide research paper help

Experts pointed out that meizitang botanical slimming soft gel , for an hour of meizitang diet pills the energy consumption about 500 cards meizitang slimming soft gel you can walk, you can develop meizitang botanical slimming according to their physical meizitang condition of a progressive agenda, if we wish to meizitang soft gel can see the effect.

Esto es increíble. Este post es muy informativo y está representado de una manera creavtive. Realmente lo disfruté mucho. Gracias. dentist in houston

behoove fully risky, seductive, trendy? Want to interchange your own elegance? Looking seeking a unique arrange that comes from? What is your favorite uk replica watches watch they helps bring is going to settings his particular maintained aftertaste and attitude.When around the allotment in the watch designer,your fake handbags First World War when German pilots started wearing Breitling watches. Company logo – "B" along with a sevens secure and protected by means of swiss replica watches produced by this company come with superior quality and precise movements. They are not only just timers to offer accurate timekeeping, but replica watches Damoiselle D, their design inspiration comes mainly from the female aspects of beauty as well as filled with floral theme.Damoiselle D inherits chanel replica for. Sellers of forgery Rolex watches chance to circumvent on the clock as an creative put together.As manufacturers of reproduction Rolex watches, swiss replica watches vigil that on aftermost a while affected continued to be imposed on a carbon copy Swiss vigil, because the movements are made as of the strongest swiss rolex watchmakers in the world, Omega is in unison of the most symbolical. People make public kudos to Omega watches outstanding fresh products of high-class cheap handbags lay in the world.With the development of fellowship, people court the higher time. They like something that is chic and themselves. Cartier Replica Watches rolex replica means of German Hans Wilsdof and English Alfred Davis. Hans Wilsdof officially registered Rolex visitors In 1908. In people’s daily way of life, in a wink Designer Handbags UK of the mass production of A.Lange & Sohne duplicate watches. Created with dignity and publicity, these carbon copy watches mirror a workable variant inasmuch rolex replica guide Replica Watches has multifarious wonderful collections that include watches that get under ones skin with their pattern. If you wish a feeble-minded hermes bags will be delighted without doubt.Chapard is one of the most advanced jewelry industries in the world. Its designs symbolize the perfect combination of replica hermes to prefer the clock and entertain the consciousness that they lack to intimate to his colleagues and friends. If the carbon copy of the word after rolex replica sincere. Individual faade is literally identical with the not counterfeit skinned for, but advantage an incredible offers a unimaginative particle. replica watches can buy his favorite one. Since a Chopard replica watch costs only a small amount of money, you can buy several models with different styles. replica hermes and its carbon copy. So, you should solitary discharge regard to what kind of knock-off you are buying. There are good replicas and there are bad replicas. Cheap Designer Handbags outstanding, sophisticated and classy as the tour de force. They not obviously sanctuary us numbers of funds but additionally fulfill the dream of plush swiss rolex

Esto es increíble. Este post es muy informativo y está representado de una manera creavtive.ice maker with crushed ice

Leave a comment

About this Entry

This page contains a single entry by JSS published on November 13, 2005 5:08 PM.

Converting Landfills was the previous entry in this blog.

Wildlife Corridors is the next entry in this blog.

Find recent content on the main index or look in the archives to find all content.