Teaching dogs to sniff out
corpses or drugs or bombs has traditionally been more craft than science. But
some novel synthetic substances may soon change that.
copyright Jessica Snyder Sachs, as originally published in Discover magazine
FOUR YEARS AGO, standing under the Arizona sun, Detective
Mark Green thought about the search ahead and felt a little queasy. Four eyewitnesses
had each told the police a similar tale of young children murdered, eight years
earlier, on a moonlit desert night. On this day the Phoenix police would search
for their remains, reportedly buried somewhere on this desolate plateau
southwest of
the city.
His partner, Green remembers, was far more enthusiastic--his shiny brown coat
was twitching with excitement, his tail whacking against Green's leg as they
stood side by side. Judge, a chocolate Labrador retriever, was accustomed to
sniffing out dope, but recently he'd been learning a new scent, that of a
human corpse. His education, though, was somewhat experimental: he had learned
this scent not from real bodies but from an artificially concocted perfume that
purportedly captured the smell
of
death.
Green now broke open an ampoule of
that perfume and gave Judge a whiff. "Back!" he commanded--as in, "Here's what
I want; now bring it back!"

Judge traced a switchback pattern across the baked red earth, his nose jumping
like a rabbit's. He paused to smell
the flattened remains of
something furry, then moved on. After 30 minutes, he slowed, swept his snout
back and forth, and started furiously digging. "Good boy!" said Green, bouncing
a red chew-toy in front of
his partner's nose. The Labrador bounded away with his reward.
Green brushed over Judge's scratch marks and took the dog several hundred yards
downwind to repeat the search. Within a few minutes Judge returned to the same
spot, again scratching and barking. Now the humans dug. They found old diapers
and shreds of
rotted clothing.
Unfortunately, the site was ground zero for an overpopulated pack-rat colony.
It looked as if any flesh and bone, had it been here, had been eaten or carried
away. Still, the human odor remained, according to Judge, who returned to claw,
bark, and bite at the unearthed clumps of clay.
A forensic pathologist from the University of Arizona in Tucson
arrived and pointed out a subtle depression in the desert floor. Not natural,
he said, but more like the settling that would follow the filling of a
wide hole. The digging continued with backhoes, and the police combed through
the clay for more evidence. One of
the alleged victims, a preschooler, had been described as wearing cowboy boots.
Even if the leather was gone, the metal shanks should have remained. The police
found nothing, but by finding those scraps of cloth, Judge became
one of
the first dogs in the ranks of
the pseudoscent-trained.
In the wake of
the Oklahoma City bombing and the Kobe earthquake, sniffing dogs have become a
common sight on television. What the pictures don't communicate, though, is how
difficult it is to train a dog to track a given scent. The dogs have to be
worked at least once a week, and if the scent in question is that of a
corpse, the trainer's life can get complicated. Carrying around the odor-laden
ooze from a corpse is not a great way to win friends. "When I'm on a three-day
trek in the desert, the real stuff will get me kicked out of camp pretty quick,"
says Green. Even training a dog to recognize a drug like heroin is problematic.
To acquire and use illegal drugs, a trainer has to plow through mountains of
paperwork; moreover, a dog can easily overdose if it gets a snoutful of the
stuff.
Trainers have therefore tried to replace the real stuff with substitutes. "For
heroin and cocaine, we mixed up a paste of powdered milk, vinegar, and a little
quinine," says Texan Billy Smith, who began training drug-sniffing dogs in the
1970s. Similarly, dogs slated for search-and- rescue missions are trained on
everything from roadkill to hair and nail clippings to their trainers' own
blood. Sometimes the substitutes work. But just as often, they don't.
A small cadre of
chemists and biologists believe that science can make the training of dogs
easier and more reliable. Their most visible handiwork, commercially available
pseudoscent, is manufactured by the Sigma Chemical Company in St. Louis. Over
the past five years, Sigma has developed a unique product line that now
includes Pseudo Corpse I (for a body less than 30 days old), Pseudo Corpse II
(a formulation designed to mimic the dry-rot scent cadavers attain after a
month), Pseudo Distressed Body, and Pseudo Drowned Victim. Pseudo Burn Victim
is in the planning stage. Sigma also sells a pseudo powder explosive and a line
of
pseudo illegal drugs.
Pseudoscent Variety Pack

"In theory, the pseudoscent is the way to go," says Larry Myers, a sensory
biologist and veterinarian at Auburn University in Alabama, "because the truly
difficult thing about training a dog to a scent is stimulus control." The ideal
compound, he says, should capture an odor signature common to everything you
want a dog to find, but nothing else. "You don't want a dog trained to find
explosives hitting on a can of
shaving cream." Even given the amazing sensitivity of a dog's sense of smell,
such things do happen. For example, Myers tells of a narcotics officer
who had trained his dog on drugs kept in plastic storage bags. "I'll be damned
if that dog didn't start alerting to the scent of Ziploc bags," says
Myers. A dog trained on street drugs can likewise get distracted by cutting
agents, homing in on baking powder in the fridge and ignoring uncut cocaine in
the pantry.
"Reliability is crucial," says Myers, "because today search dogs are being used
in life-and-death
situations." Among those who rely on such dogs is the Federal Aviation
Administration, which deploys roughly 100 canine search teams to check
suspicious-looking air cargo for explosives. The faa might be interested in
using pseudoexplosives in the future--one reason being that real explosives
have a nasty way of
actually exploding-- and so it sponsors research on dog olfaction, including
Myers's. But before he or anyone else is going to be able to come up with a
reliable pseudo bomb scent, Myers says, there's a lot of basic science that
needs to be discovered.
Researchers know that when a dog sniffs deeply and odor-carrying molecules flow
into its nasal cavity, the shape of
the cavity changes so that the molecules are focused onto a yellow, rippled,
mucus-covered membrane, called the sensory mucosa, toward the back of the
snout. So convoluted is the canine mucosa that if it were smoothed flat it
would be several times larger than the dog's head. Because it has so much
surface area, the mucosa can carry a vast number of odor-sensitive,
hairlike cilia- -ten times more than are found in a human nose.
But beyond that, researchers know very little. They have yet, for instance, to
define the limits of
a dog's sense of
smell.
A dog may be able to track the day-old trail of a fugitive, yet
when it comes to certain smells, such as that of acetone (the sweet smell of nail
polish), a dog's nose is no better than a human's. No one has yet
systematically sorted out just what a dog can smell and exactly how
it does so.
Against this background of
meager knowledge, Sigma chemists Thomas Juehne and John Revell created their
first pseudoscents in 1989. Dog handlers working for federal agencies had come
to Sigma asking for compounds for training narcotics dogs. Revell began with
heroin and cocaine, each of
which consists of
a single big complex molecule. "With such pure, large compounds," he explains,
"we knew we had to find some outer piece to work with, a little active site
that might break off from the main molecule." Such a piece would probably be
safe--that is, nonnarcotic--yet present in the air around the drug, so a dog
could be trained with it to recognize the drug.
Fortunately, U.S. Customs had already done a lot of Revell and Juehne's
work for them, analyzing the gases that float above both heroin and cocaine and
isolating a variety of
alcohols, alkanes, esters, and acids. All were readily available in Sigma's
catalog of
35,000 laboratory chemicals. Revell and Juehne could proceed directly to a game
of
mix and match: they developed several test batches for each drug and sent them
to six handlers with dogs already trained on real narcotics. Each handler was
asked to try to have their dogs find a hidden sample. The dogs completely
ignored some samples while showing keen interest in others, and from these
Sigma created refined formulas.

Search dogs assist diving team in body recovery.
After a confirmation round with the veteran dogs, Revell sent the most
promising signature for each drug to a second set of handlers, asking
them to use it to train new dogs not yet exposed to the scent of actual drugs.
Reports came back that these pseudo-trained dogs were then able to locate the
real stuff. VoilĂ : Sigma had its first pseudos.
Developing a pseudomarijuana has been more complicated, says Revell. "Instead of a
single pure compound, now we're working with a whole plant." To isolate the
molecules in marijuana and determine their abundance, he uses both gas
chromatography, which can separate chemicals based in part on how quickly they
evaporate from a liquid, and mass spectrometry, which identifies compounds
according to their atomic mass and charge.
Revell looks in particular for substances that will become gaseous even at low
temperatures, since these would be the compounds most likely to waft from a
hidden stash. "Unfortunately, we discovered that not all dogs alert to the same
thing," he says. Though all the dogs had been trained on whole marijuana, they
had apparently selected different signature chemicals to use for
identification. Revell was able to produce a commercial pseudomarijuana by
taking several of
the most popular compounds and combining them in a scent cocktail, on which all
the dogs hit. Still, he wants to tinker with the formula more, since Sigma has
received occasional reports of
the cocktail's not working. "The first came from Saudi Arabia," says Revell.
"My hunch is there may be differences between marijuana varieties worldwide."
In 1990 dog handlers let Sigma know about the troubles they had training their
dogs on corpses. Because this type of work comes in irregular spurts,
handlers need to train their dogs continually--at least once a week, preferably
more. Their substance of
choice is dirt collected from under a corpse, which becomes infused with its
putrid smell.
Reflecting the callousness probably essential to the job, the handlers refer to
this training aid by a number of
names: "dirty dirt," "Mr. Sousa," or "Fred," as in "Fred B. Dead." Nobody likes
handling the stuff. Trainer Carl Makins, of the Greenville, South Carolina,
sheriff's office, keeps his double wrapped in plastic and locked inside a
vapor-proof munitions cache. When he opens the box even for a second, he
saturates the room with a sickeningly sweet smell. (Think skunk
meets Montezuma's revenge.) But that's the least of a trainer's
worries--there's also the threat of
infection associated with hiv, hepatitis, and other diseases transmitted
through body fluids.
Hearing such complaints, Patricia Carr, Sigma's liaison to the dog handlers,
went to Revell and Juehne and said, "Give me body in a bottle." At first they
looked at Carr as if she were crazy, but eventually they warmed to the idea.
That's not to say that they allowed any ooze in their lab, let alone in their
gas chromatograph. "That would have been difficult for me," Juehne says with an
audible shudder. Instead he searched through scientific journals and found that
the human body had been well quantified in various states of decomposition.
Five to fifteen minutes after death,
protein synthesis in the body grinds to a halt. With nothing to maintain the
protective lining of
the gut, digestive enzymes eat the body from the inside out, splitting proteins
into amino acids. At the same time, the body's resident bacteria, unhindered by
an immune system, feast on the amino acids and skyrocket in number. As the
bacteria produce chemicals such as ammonia and ptomaines (with such apt names
as putrescine and cadaverine), they produce the distinctive smell of decaying flesh.
Each stage of
decomposition produces distinct peaks and ebbs in the levels of various chemicals,
including the ptomaines, which is a great help to both the pathologist who
wants to determine the time of
death
and the chemist trying to emulate the smell of it.
Juehne cataloged the chemicals most likely to be in the air or soil around a
decaying human body--both fresh (Pseudo Corpse I) and well aged (Pseudo Corpse
II). Among these, he looked for chemicals that might set the smell of a human corpse
apart from that of
an animal. "I needed something unique about the human body versus a dead
animal," says Juehne.
Juehne's preliminary guinea pig was Revell. Revell had joined up with Sigma
after seven years in a forensics lab, where he often worked alongside coroners
at autopsies and crime scenes. "Basically," says Revell, "once Tom had a list of
potential compounds, he began running them by my nose and asking, 'Does this smell
like a corpse?' I'd say, 'Yeah, that's close,' and he'd disappear back into his
lab to refine it."
Juehne diluted his scents to a level indiscernible to humans and sent them to a
half-dozen dog handlers. The first batch was well received; a more refined brew
drew raves. "I started out their biggest skeptic," says Billy Smith, "but as
soon as we hid this stuff in a sandbar, the gators stole it. Then we put some
in a tree, and the coons stole it; in a log stump, and the buzzards stole it."
Another tester was Caroline Hebard, a New Jersey mother of four who has been
honored internationally for her canine search-and-rescue work. "Yes, this
works," she told Sigma. "Now give me something for live folk."
But not just ordinary live folk. Over the years, as Hebard and her dogs sifted
through the rubble of
earthquakes and explosions, she saw that she needed to train her dogs to tell
her if they were smelling buried trauma victims or the workers around them.
"There's a certain scent, kind of
sour and sweaty, around someone in shock," she says. "Anybody who's familiar
with the smell
in an ambulance knows what I mean." To fill the request, Juehne hit the
journals again. There he found detailed analyses of compounds our
bodies secrete onto the surface of
our skin. "I needed to find a universal human scent, something
nondiscriminatory with respect to a person's diet, sex, or age--from that
baby-fresh smell
of
a newborn's head to the musty odor of Grandpa in the nursing home."
After another game of
chemical mix and match, Juehne sent Pseudo Distress for field-testing. It
reportedly sailed through all trials, with claims that dogs trained on the
stuff were proving their worth in actual rescue situations. And not only did
Pseudo Distress help dogs track people in shock: handlers report that it's good
for finding frightened children in the wilderness and adrenaline-charged
escapees in prison air ducts.
The company added its most recent pseudoscent--Drowned Victim--by reformulating
its corpse tinctures into a granulated capsule that sinks in water. "The first
batch was like Alka-Seltzer," comments Hebard. "It had the dogs jumping to bite
overhanging branches." What trainers needed was a scent that would collect in a
thin film just on top of
the water's surface- -as true cadaver scent does--so dogs could follow its concentration
gradient to the source. Accordingly, researchers at Sigma made a slower-
dissolving capsule and filled it partly with salt grains to make it sink. "That
did the trick," says Hebard.
For all the testimonials to the pseudoscents' effectiveness, there is still
plenty of
room for skepticism. There are no statistics from a controlled test of
pseudoscents with large numbers of
handlers who themselves did not know where the samples were hidden. Nor has the
accuracy of
dogs trained on pseudos been reliably compared with that of dogs trained on the
real thing.
"We need to separate the science from the mumbo jumbo," says Myers. To begin
with, he says, nobody yet knows what a dog is physiologically capable of
smelling. A simple analysis of
a drug or a decaying body won't tell you which chemicals are of canine interest.
That question is among those Myers is trying to answer at Auburn's Institute
for Biological Detection Systems. Myers founded the institute in 1989 to study
everything from actual canaries in coal mines to microbes that glow when
exposed to pollutants. But for now, 90 percent of his grant money
arrives earmarked for studying canine detectors.
The work begins in the sensory lab: wrapped in a baby-blue blindfold, a tan
cocker mix lies on a padded table. Two students murmur reassuringly as they
clip to the dog's scalp electrodes that will pick up general patterns of
brain activity when she is presented with a test smell. They also focus
an overhead camera on her head. One student then lifts a test tube suspended
from a two-foot glass handle. As the tube nears the cocker's nose, an
electroencephalograph across the room traces eight jagged lines to record a
spark of
brain activity. The overhead camera captures the slight movements of a
sniff.
Myers's students are determining the limits of the cocker's sense of smell
with a dilution of
eugenol, one of
the odor-producing molecules in cloves. Myers employs eugenol as a standard for
determining whether his test dogs are having an off day, since dogs, like
people, experience a range of
colds and allergies that can interfere with smell.
If the cocker's sense of
smell
is up to snuff, the students test her ability to detect vanishingly small
amounts of
an explosive and then several of
its volatile ingredients. Ultimately, Myers would like to isolate just one or
two key chemicals that dogs can use to recognize the entire explosive. The
result could be a safe, reliable pseudo.
Enlisted in the effort are two men that Myers admiringly calls the institute's
control freaks: chemist Mark Hartell, an eager young doctoral student with a
passion for ferreting out contaminants, and experimental psychologist Jim
Johnston. Skinnerian to the bone, Johnston is likewise obsessed with purging
contaminants--the type that creep in when humans bring subjectivity to the
study of
dog behavior.
To begin with, says Hartell, "what's in the list of ingredients is not
necessarily what's in the air around an explosive. If the guy down the hall is
wearing Polo, that doesn't mean the explosive you're studying is made by Ralph
Lauren." Today Hartell is fueling his gas chromatograph and mass spectrometer
with air drawn from the explosive under study. He's already identified dozens of
airborne compounds, many of
which were contaminants from the institute's house air. ("Dirty stuff," he
comments.) Many of
the other compounds break down too quickly for a dog to notice. That leaves a
dozen or so worth examining.
The researchers use conditioning experiments to test these remaining chemicals.
Their subject dogs do their work in the isolation of six wooden
chambers--oversize Skinner boxes--in a room slung with computer wires and
plastic air tubes. No human handlers here. "Uncontrollable variable," says
Johnston--humans have a habit of
unconsciously affecting the response of dogs by subtle changes in their
appearance. Johnston prefers the objectivity of a computer program.
Each chamber is equipped with a nose cup attached to an olfactometer, a
glorified air pump that delivers a precisely calibrated flow of clean or scented
air. Inside the chamber, slightly above the cup, are three levers. The dogs
have been trained to press the right lever when they smell the explosive
under study, the left lever when they get a puff of clean air, and the
middle lever when the air contains a scent other than the explosive.
In chamber two, a white shepherd named Columbus begins her eight- hundredth
session. (Each dog works one hour a day.) At the sound of a tone, she inserts
her snout in the cup. At a second tone, she removes it and paws the middle
lever: in other words, she smells something, but not one of the explosives she
has been trained on.
So far, admits Johnston, none of
the dogs trained to recognize the explosive have responded to any one isolated
ingredient. But only a few chemicals have been tested as yet. If no one
ingredient evokes a response, they will try two- or three-chemical mixtures.
Developing a pseudoscent in this way is time-consuming, Myers admits, but it
may help reveal the classes of
chemicals to which dogs are most sensitive. To know whether dogs are indeed
more attuned to certain compounds, and to identify which ones, would elevate
canine training to a new and reliable height.
Till then, 1,850 dog handlers will continue to use Sigma's fascinating but
sketchily tested perfumes. According to the company's customer logs, sales have
more than doubled in the last three years. Often the trainers who buy them use
them in combination with more traditional materials. "I like to mix it up,"
says Smith, who trains dogs initially with corpse pseudoscent, then graduates
them to dirty dirt. Hebard combines pseudoscent with human hair for "a very
strong response."
Many handlers, though, steer clear of the scents. "Using pseudos is like
going to the firing range with blanks," argues David Frost, canine training
supervisor for the Tennessee Public Service Commission. "The strongest thing we
have going for us is the dog's amazing power to discriminate one thing from
another. So why muck that up with anything but the real thing?" Using real
drugs means leaping over a long series of bureaucratic hurdles, he admits, "but
sometimes to do something right isn't convenient."
Still, the stories of
success linger and tantalize. Out in Arizona, Detective Frank Shenkowitz, who
inherited Green's grisly case, remains haunted. "I still go back there fairly
often," he says of
the plateau where the pseudoscent-trained Labrador Judge uncovered the decayed
clothes. "You never know what the desert is going to toss back at you." A while
ago, not far from Judge's find, Shenkowitz came across a tiny faded cowboy
boot--the size a four-year-old might wear. "It doesn't prove murder," he says.
"But I know Judge is reliable."
Science writer Jessica Snyder Sachs is the author of Good
Germs, Bad Germs: Health and Survival in a Bacterial World (Hill&Wang/FSG)
and Corpse: Nature, Forensics, and the Struggle to Pinpoint Time of Death
(Perseus Books).